Wednesday, February 01, 2006

Cultural Differences In Cognition: The Case of Fish

A few times a week, I get an email from a blog reader asking me a cognitive science question. They ask about a lot of different topics, but there are a few topics on which I get questions regularly. Two of the most frequent are Evolutionary Psychology and Lakoff, and I can only blame myself for that. A third frequent question topic, however, is something that I haven't discussed much, so it must be a topic in which a lot of people are really interested: cultural differences in cognition. It's always difficult answering questions about cultural differences, because there are a lot of different areas of research on cultural differences, and at the same time, there's not a lot of research on cultural differences (meaning that people are studying differences in a lot of different areas, they just haven't gotten very far in doing so). Many of the questions are inspired by The Geography of Thought, and thus are about analytical vs. holistic/dialectical thinking. I've talked about that a little in the past. Others, though fewer, surprisingly (having been around enough students, I know this is something a lot of people wonder about) are about linguistic relativity and the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. I've talked about that a little, too. But no one ever asks about what is, to me, the most interesting research on cultural differences, work on cultural differences in categorization. That tells me that most people don't really know about that research, and that means I need to write a post about it. Or at least, that gives me an excuse to write one. And it just so happens that I read a paper on the topic earlier this week that provides a nice look at that research. So, here's a post about it.

First a little background. While research on cultural differences in cognition is very sexy these days, and is in fact older than cognitive science itself (e.g., the work on memory by Bartlett), for about the last 30 years, cognitive psychology, and the study of concepts and categories in particular, has been dominated by objectivism. In the study of concepts and categories, this view was first articulated by Eleanor Rosch1, who argued that our categories "carve the world at its joints," or more technically, "that correlated features or properties create natural 'chunks' or basic level categories that any well-adapted categorization system must acknowledge or exploit"2. The implication of this idea is that everyone, regardless of their cultural background, will have pretty much the same categories, because they're exposed to pretty much the same correlational structure of features in the world. And to a large extent, empirical research has borne this idea out, especially for natural categories like animals and plants3, but even for some artifacts4.

Research over the last decade or so has begun to challenge the objectivist view of concepts. Central to the objectivist view is the idea of a "basic level," which is the level at which within-category similarity is high while between-category similarity is low. Examples of basic level categories include BIRD, CAR, CHAIR, and FISH. Other levels of categorization might admit some cultural variability, under the objectivist view. This is the level that is supposed to be the most constricted by the world's joints. But in the early 90s, researchers produced evidence that the Basic Level shifts with expertise. When people are experts in a particular domain, they tend to treat subordinate level categories (ROBIN, VW BEETLE, DESK CHAIR, GROUPER; categories in which within-level similarity is higher than at the basic level, but between-category similarity is higher as well) like basic level categories. This means that how we perceive the world's correlational structure depends, to some extent, on our knowledge. Along this line, research on theory-based theories of concepts, which argue that things like knowledge of causal relations, in addition to correlational structure, factor in to how we divide the world into categories.

So, with the recognition that knowledge influences categorization, it's possible to start thinking about cultural differences, since knowledge across cultures will certainly differ to some extent. In order to explore potential differences, then, Doug Medin and his colleagues have studied the differences in categorization among different kinds of experts and novices. For example, when categorizing trees, landscapers tend to sort them differently than park maintenance workers and botanists. The latter two groups' sortings tend to agree with scientific taxonomies, as do the sortings of novices, while tree landscapers appear to be using knowledge related to their use of trees (goal-related knowledge) to classify them5. This implies that different cultural groups that have different sets of goals related to the same objects may classify them differently.

Recent research also suggests that there may be cultural differences in categorization that are not due to goal-related knowledge. Atran et al.6 found that three neighboring tribes in a Guatemalan rainforest with similar agrarian lifestyles, and thus similar goal-related knowledge bases, classified plants and animals similarly, but reasoned about them differently. In other words, while their categories divide the world at about the same joints, their concept representations contain different information. It may be, then, that even when members of different cultures classify things in the same way, they actually have very different representations of those things.

In order to explore this, Medin et al.7 conducted a series of experiments comparing the ways in which two expert populations, American fisherman of European ancestry (majority culture) living in Wisconsin and and Menominee Indian fishermen in Wisconsin, categorize and represent freshwater fish. Both populations have similar goal-related knowledge of fish, and live in the same region, so any differences observed can't be attributed to goal-related knowledge from expertise. In the first experiment, both groups were given the names of 44 different species of local fish, and asked list the properties of each species and then to sort them into "meaningful categories," and to provide reasons for sorting them the way they did. They found that the sortings of the two groups were similar, but different in ways that corresponded with the types of justifications that they gave. For example, the majority culture participants used goal-related categories such as food and garbage fish, that the Menominee participants did not have. The Menominee participants, on the other hand, had categories based on the fish's habitat, whereas the majority culture participants did not. These sorting differences were reflected in the justifications the two groups gave, with both groups giving goal-related justifications, but the Menominee giving ecological (e.g., habitat-related) justifications much more often than the majority group participants.

In order to explore these differences in more depth, in a subsequent experiment, Medin et al. asked participants from the two groups to describe interactions between the different species of fish. They found that both groups had a great deal of knowledge of the interactions between different species of fish, but that the types of interactions they described different significantly. The Menominee participants were much more likely to mention two types of interactions: positive interactions, in which one species helps another, and reciprocal interactions, in which two species benefit each other, than were majority participants. These differences in representations are not likely to be due to expertise, since both groups have similar levels of knowledge of the relationships between species, and they're not likely to be goal related, since they both have similar goals (catching the fish). So the differences must be due to some other aspect of culture. Medin et al. offer the following explanation:
Our speculation is that cultural attitudes and beliefs reinforce certain “habits of mind” or characteristic ways of thinking about some domain. Specifically, responses of majority culture informants concerning ecological relations may be filtered through a goal-related framework whereas the responses of the Menominee informants appear to be less “viewer-centered.” (p. 44)
They argue that these differences in focus or orientation will make some types information more or less available to the members of the different groups. Thus, the two groups may have the same knowledge base, but some parts of that knowledge base will be easier to access, depending on culturally-induced focus. This will lead the groups to activate different information from their knowledge base when questions are more general. In order to test this explanation, Medin et al. conducted a third experiment in which they directly elicited participants' knowledge of the relationships between the different species of fish, by asking them to sort them on the basis of habitat. By directly eliciting this kind of information, it should be easily accessible even when participants' cultural focus makes this information less accessible in other contexts (e.g., that of the previous experiment). Consistent with this, they found that both groups were able to reliably sort based on habitat. Thus, it does appear that the two groups have the same information in their knowledge base, but that Menomineeinee participants could access ecological information more easily than the majority group participants.

So, the Medin et al. studies, combined with the studies of different types of experts, and the neighboring tribes in Guatemala, all imply that cultural differences in how we categorize and conceptualize things do exist. These differences are likely due to differences in how we interact with those things (i.e., differences in our goals), as well as to cultural differences in focus or perspective. It's important to note, however, that while the differences in our conceptual representations can be big, and important for how we reason, ultimately, the differences in categorization, or how we divide the world up, are pretty small. As Medin et al. put it, cultural differences "emerge against a backdrop of similarity" (p. 58). This similarity is likely due to the correlational structure of the world that motivates the objectivist view. So, Medin et al. state, "cultural models and biological reality both 'bend' a bit in answering one another's demands, and so determining folk conceptions of freshwater fish" (p. 59).

The picture painted by these studies is the one that I try to give when answering questions about cultural differences in cognition in general. The fact is, regardless of where we live, and in what cultures we are raised, we all have highly similar perceptual experiences, and come into the world with the same perceptual and cognitive mechanisms. Thus, for the most part, the way we think about things will be pretty similar, especially for natural categories. However, differences in knowledge across cultures, related to goals and expertise, as well as differences in perspective (Medin et al.'s "cultural models"), will inevitably create differences in the representations we use in processing that information, how accessible certain information is, and as a result, how we reason about things.

1 E.g., Rosch, E. (1974). Linguistic relativity. In A. Silverstein (Ed.), Human Communication: Theoretical Perspectives. New York: Halsted Press; & Rosch, E. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382-439.
2 Medin, D.L, Ross, N., Atran, S., Cox, D., Wakaua, H.J., Coley, J.D., Proffitt, J.B. & Blok, S. (in press). The Role of Culture in the Folkbiology of Freshwater Fish. Cognitive Psychology, p. 2.
3E.g., Boster, J.S. & DÂ’Andrade, R. (1989) Natural and human sources of cross-cultural agreement in ornithological classification. American Anthropologist, 91, 132-142.
4 Malt, B. C. (1995). Category coherence in cross-cultural perspective. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 85-148.
5 Medin, D. L., Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome? Cognitive Psychology, 32, 49-96.
6 Atran, S., Medin, D.L., Ross, N., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ucan EkÂ’,E., Coley, J., Timura, C. & Baran, M. (2002). Folkecology, Cultural Epidemiology and the Spirit of the Commons: A Garden Experiment in the Maya Lowlands. Current Anthropology, 41, 1-23.
7 Medin, D.L, Ross, N., Atran, S., Cox, D., Wakaua, H.J., Coley, J.D., Proffitt, J.B. & Blok, S. (in press). The Role of Culture in the Folkbiology of Freshwater Fish. Cognitive Psychology.

11 comments:

Razib Khan said...

nice post. i enjoyed nisbett's book, but i wasn't convinced that the differences were that deep when at the end he shows how people in different cultures can be 'trained' to think alternatively in a few hours.

Chris said...

razib, right, and the research I discussed in the post on the Geography of Thought stuff (the link way up at the top of this post) discusses experiments in which American college students were induced to "think" like East Asians by increasing their fear of isolation.

That's really why I like the research on cultural differences in concepts and categorization, because it really gets to the heart of what underlies them. You could call the differences "heuristics" or "biases," if you wanted, though I like Medin's "cultural models" and I usually call it differences in "focus," but they all amount to the same thing: the accessibility of information and the representations used to process information. And the usefulness of the research on cultural differences, in my mind, is in showing how important those things are.

Chris Chatham said...

I'm with Razib - nice post.

So I think the natural question is, what do these cultural differences in categorization really reflect? My take on it, from what little I've read, is that they tell us more about cultural differences than they do about categorization.

Not to belittle the importance of cultural differences, of course. In fact, I was just introduced to Gordon's paper in Science describing the Piraha tribe, who apparently can't count past 2! There's been lots of debate on this finding, and in the end I've just been amazed that the Piraha have maintained a culture where they have no need for conceptions of exact quantity.

Fido the Yak said...

Anybody ever read Mimica's Intimations of Infinity? I recall it being pretty deep, but it's been many years. Think I'll pick it up again, and also take a look at Gary Urton's The Social Life of Numbers.

Do these kinds of ethnographic studies really tell us about categorization? I think at a minimum they provide evidence of plasticity in what are thought to be basic structures of mind, and that should be highly relevant to certain views of categorization.

Chris said...

Fido, I think they also tell us a lot about the relevance of things like goals, background knowledge, and schemas for categorization. Traditionally (and by that, I mean for the last 30+ years), the dominant similarity-based models have either ignored those things or haven't done a good job of accounting for them.

usman said...

Could you please me to find some stuff related to cross-cultural differences in food categorization?

Kind Regards

Cheryl said...

Research over the last decade or so has begun to challenge the objectivist view of concepts.
Learn how to speak French and learn speak Chinese

Term Papers said...

Fido,Its interesting issue and I think they also tell us a lot about the relevance of things like goals, background knowledge, and schemas for categorization.


Term

papers

Anonymous said...

bag
bags
handbag
handbags
Coach new bags
Chanel new bags
D&G new bags
Burberry new bags
Prada new bags
ED Hardy new bags
CHOLE new bags
Fendi new bags
Dooney&Bourke new bags
Juicy new bags
LV new bags
Gucci new bags

Anonymous said...

sunglasses outlet
air max,Nike air max
air max 90
nike air max 90
air max 1
air max 95
nike air max 360
nike air max ltd
air max ltd
air max 360
air max classic,Air Max Huarache
air max light
air max bw
air max plus
air max 180
air max nike
air max TN
air max 87
air max 2010
air max outlet
timberland
timberland boots
timberland shoes
cheap timberland boots
timberland uk
timberlands
timberland 2010
timberland outlet

Anonymous said...

Armani Sunglasses Burberry Sunglasses Cartier Sunglasses
Chanel Sunglasses
Coach Sunglasses
D&G Sunglasses
Dior Sunglasses
ED Hardy Sunglasses
Fendi Sunglasses
LV Sunglasses
Oakley Sunglasses
Police Sunglasses
Ray Ban Sunglasses
Roberto Cavalli Sunglasses
Versace Sunglasses
cheap gucci sunglasses
cheap prada sunglasses
cheap D&G sunglasses
discount gucci sunglasses
discount prada sunglasses
discotunt D&G sunglasses
cheap oaklay sunglasses
discount oaklay sunglasses